Untitled Document

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

> ¿¬±¸¼Ò¼Ò°³ > ´ë»ó°ü°èÀÌ·Ð

 

´ë»ó°ü°èÀ̷а¡µéÀº Á¤Åë ÇÁ·ÎÀ̵å ÇÐÆÄÀÇ ¼º°Ý ¸ðµ¨°ú´Â ´Ù¸¥ ¹æ¹ýÀ¸·Î °ü°è¿Í ¼º°ÝÀÇ ±¸Á¶¿Í ±× ±¸Á¶Àû ¹ß´Þ¿¡ ´ëÇØ ¿¬±¸ÇØ ¿Ô´Ù.
À̵éÀº ´ë»ó°ü°è À̷а¡¿Í Àڱ⠽ɸ®ÇÐ À̷а¡·Î ºÐ·ùµÈ´Ù. ´ë»ó°ü°è¿Í Àڱ⠽ɸ®ÇÐ À̷а¡µéÀº ¸ðµÎ ÀÚ½ÅÀ» Á¤½ÅºÐ¼®ÇÐÀÇ ÁÖ·ù³»¿¡ ÀÖ´Â °ÍÀ¸·Î °£ÁÖÇÏÁö¸¸ ±×µéÀÇ Áß¿äÇÑ ÀÌ·ÐÀû ºÎºÐ¿¡ À־´Â ±× È帧À» ´Þ¸®ÇÑ´Ù.

 

´ë»ó°ü°èÀ̷а¡µéÀº ÃʱâÀÇ Á¤½Å±¸Á¶¿Í ½É¸®±âÁ¦ÀÇ Çü¼º°úÁ¤¿¡¼­ ¹ßÇöµÇ´Â °¨º°(differentiation)À» ¿¬±¸ÇÏ°í ÀÌ·¯ÇÑ ³»Àû ±¸Á¶µéÀÌ Çö½ÇÀûÀÎ ´ëÀΰü°è »óȲ¿¡¼­ ¾î¶»°Ô ÀçÇöµÇ´Â°¡¸¦ Á¶»çÇÑ´Ù. À̵é À̷а¡µéÀº Áö¼ÓÀûÀÎ Àλó Áï, °³ÀÎÀÇ Á¤½Å³»¿¡ Á¸ÀçÇÏ´Â ÀÜÀç(residue), ȤÀº Â±â(remnant)¸¦ ³²±â´Â »ýÈÄ Ãʱ⠰ü°è¿¡ ÃÊÁ¡À» ¸ÂÃá´Ù. ÀÌ·¯ÇÑ °ú°Å °ü°èÀÇ ÀÜÀç, ´Ù¸¥ ¸»·Î ³»Àû ´ë»ó°ü°è°¡ °³ÀÎÀÇ Àνİú ´Ù¸¥ °³Àεé°úÀÇ °ü°èÀÇ ÇüŸ¦ ¸¸µé±â ¶§¹®ÀÌ´Ù.
Àΰ£Àº ±×°¡ °Å·¡ÇÏ´Â ½ÇÁ¦ÀÇ Å¸Àεé°úÀÇ °ü°è¸¦ »ç½Ç ±×´ë·Î ȤÀº »ç½ÇÀ» ¿Ö°îÇØ Çؼ® (¼³¸í)ÇÏ´Â ³»ºÎÀÇ Å¸ÀÎ, Áï Á¤½ÅÀûÀΠǥ»óÀ̶ó°í ĪÇÏ´Â ¼ºÀå°úÁ¤¿¡¼­ ¿µ»óÈ­µÈ ´ë»óÀÇ À̹ÌÁö¿Í °ü°èÇÏ´Â °ÍÀÌ´Ù.

 

´ë»ó°ü°èÀÌ·ÐÀº Ŭ¶óÀÎ (Melanie Klein)¿¡ ÀÇÇؼ­ â½ÃµÇ¾ú´Ù. KleinÀº Ç×°¡¸®ÀÇ ºÎ´ÙÆ佺Ʈ¿¡¼­ ž ÃʵîÇб³ ±³»ç¸¦ Áö³»´Ù°¡ FruedÀÇ Á¤½ÅºÐ¼®Çп¡ ¸Å·áµÇ¾î ºóÀ¸·Î °Ç³Ê°¡ FruedÀÇ Á¦ÀÚ°¡ µÇ¾ú´Ù. ±×ÈÄ ±×³àÀº ·±´øÀ¸·Î ÀÌÁÖÇÏ¿© ¾Æµ¿Á¤½ÅºÐ¼®°¡ µÇ¾ú´Ù.
1930³â´ë¿Í 40³â´ë¿¡ ±×³à´Â ½ºÄ«Æ²·£µå¿¡ ÀÖ´Â ¿¡µò¹ö·¯ÀÇ
Æä¾îº£¾ð (W. R. D. Fairbairn)°ú »ý°¢À» ±³È¯Çϸ鼭 ¼­·Î°£¿¡ ¿µÇâÀ» ÁÖ¾ú´Ù. ±×µéÀº °°Àº ½Ã±â¿¡ ´ë»ó°ü°èÀÌ·ÐÀ̶ó´Â »õ·Î¿î ÁöÆòÀ» ¿­¾úÀ¸³ª Æä¾îº£¾ð¿¡ ÀÇÇØ ±× ¿ãÁ°¡ Klein¿¡°Ô ¾çµµµÇ¾ú´Ù.

·±´øÀÇ ¼Ò¾Æ°ú ÀÇ»çÀÎ
À§´ÏÄÚÆ® (D. W. Winnicott)´Â ¾Æµ¿½É¸®Ä¡·á¸¦ ÇÏ°í ÀÖ¾ú´Âµ¥ ´Ù¸¥ Á¤½ÅºÐ¼® Àú¼­¿Í´Â º°·Î °ü·Ã¼ºÀÌ ¾ø´Â
µ¶Æ¯ÇÏ°í µ¶Ã¢ÀûÀÎ Àú¼­µéÀ» ÃâÆÇÇß´Ù.


Çë°¡¸®¿¡¼­ ž ºñ¿£³ª¿¡¼­ ¼ö¾÷ÇÑ ÈÄ ´º¿åÀ¸·Î ÀÌÁÖÇØ ¾Æµ¿À» Ä¡·áÇÑ
¸¶Èê·¯ (Margaret Mahler)´Â 1950³â´ëºÎÅÍ 70³â´ë±îÁö
¸¹Àº ¿µÇâ·ÂÀÖ´Â ±Ûµé°ú Àú¼­µéÀ» ¹ßÇ¥Çß´Ù.

¶ÇÇÑ µ¶ÀÏ¿¡¼­ °Ç³Ê¿Â Á¦ÀÌÄß½¼(Edith Jacobson)µµ ÀÌ ½Ã±â¿¡ ´º¿å¿¡¼­ È°µ¿ÇÏ°í ±ÛÀ» ½è´Ù.


¿ª½Ã ºñ¿£³ª Ãâ½ÅÀÎ
ÄÁ¹ö±× (Otto Kernberg)´Â Ä¥·¹¿¡¼­ ÀÇ»ç¼ö¾÷°ú Á¤½ÅÄ¡·á ÈÆ·ÃÀ» ¹Þ°í ĵ»ç½ºÀÇ ¸Þ´×°Å Ŭ¸®´Ð¿¡¼­ °è¼Ó
Á¤½ÅÄ¡·á¸¦ ÇØ ³ª°¬´Ù. ¾Õ¿¡¼­ ¾ð±ÞÇß´ø ÇÐÀÚµéÀÇ °³³äÀ» ±â¹ÝÀ¸·Î Çؼ­ Àú¼úÇÑ ±×ÀÇ Ã¥°ú ³í¹®µéÀº 1970³â´ë¿¡ ¹ßÇ¥µÇ±â ½ÃÀÛÇß´Ù.


ºñ¿£³ª¿¡¼­ ž°í Á¤½ÅºÐ¼®¿¡ À־ ÈìÀâÀ» µ¥ ¾ø´Â °æ·ÂÀ» º¸À¯ÇÏ°í ÀÖ´Â
ÄÚȣƮ(Heinz Kohut)´Â ½ÃÄ«°í¿¡¼­ ´ëºÎºÐÀÇ Ä¡·á
È°µ¿À» Çß´Ù.
±×ÀÇ ¸í¼ºÀÌ ÀýÁ¤¿¡ ´Ù´Þ¾Ò´ø 1970³â´ë¿¡ ±×´Â Á¤½ÅºÐ¼®Çа迡 ÆĹ®À» ÀÏÀ¸Å°°í Á¤½ÅºÐ¼® »ç°íÀÇ È帧¸¦ º¯È­½ÃÄ×´ø Àڱ⠽ɸ®ÇÐÀ» ´Ù·é Ã¥µéÀ» ÃâÆÇÇß´Ù.

À̵鿡 ÀÇÇؼ­ ´ë»ó°ü°èÀÌ·ÐÀº °è¼Ó ¹ßÀüÇÏ°í º¯¸ðµÇ¾î ¿ÔÀ¸¸ç Áö±Ýµµ °è¼Ó ¹ßÀüµÇ°í ÀÖ´Â °úÁ¤¿¡ ÀÖ´Ù.

 

looksmart¿¡¼­ ãÀº ÀÚ·áµé

What is Object Relations?

»ó´ã°ü·Ã´Üü »çÀÌÆ® 

 

Carl Bagnini°¡ ¿î¿µÇÏ´Â ¿¬±¸¼ÒÀÇ ÇÁ·Î±×·¥

Michael KaufmanÀÌ ¿î¿µÇÏ´Â ¿¬±¸¼Ò ÇÁ·Î±×·¥

OBJECT RELATIONS COUPLE AND FAMILY THERAPY  ÇÁ·Î±×·¥ ¾È³»

Washington Institute of Psychiatry

 

Object Relations Theory emerges wholly from the profound impact of the work of Melanie Klein (1882-1960). Klein sought to elaborate on and extend Freud's original theory through her observations and clinical work with children. Indeed, Klein's work
as a whole is an extension of Freud's work, but also a transformation of Freud's original insights through her unique interpretive perspectives. Klein was also profoundly influenced by Sandor Ferenczi, her own psychoanalyst. Klein's insights were so transformative of Freud's work, in fact, that her theoretical work was rejected by many orthodox Freudians -- a clash best represented in the split between Klein's "London school" and the "Viennese school," most closely associated with the figure of Anna Freud. The initial class between Klein and Anna Freud, leading to this profound and lasting 'split,' involved differences in opinion regarding the treatment of children. Klein used play therapy and used interpretive techniques which were very similar to the techniques used with adults. Anna Freud, on the other hand, held that children's egos were not yet developed enough for classical analysis, and, instead, she advocated a more educative role for the analyst who works with children. The heated debates in WWII Britain -- within the British psychoanalytic society -- led to a profound schism in the psychoanalytic community which is still evident to this day. In fact, until recently, most American psychoanalysts, who were more closely aligned with Freudian ego psychology, held Klein and subsequent Object Relations Theory in contempt for this reason, and, vice versa, the Kleinian tradition generally demonized the ego psychology movement. Thankfully, today this schism is beginning to heal.

Working with children, Klein felt she had observed processes in pre-Oedipal children that were very similar to Oedipal conflicts in older children. Throughout her career, she attempted to theoretically justify these observations. In turn, Klein and her followers applied her practice and theory to work with psychotic adult patients. Klein generally saw similarities between young children's coping strategies in play and psychotic symptoms. In general, however, Klein imagined that all adults retain, at some level, such psychotic processes, involving a constant struggle to cope with paranoid anxiety and depressive anxiety. Klein was led, therefore, to apply her approach to adult neurotics, as well as psychotics and children. Klein's technique, in all cases, involved a method of using "deep" interpretations which she felt communicated directly to the unconscious of the client, thus by-passing ego defenses. In conclusion, Klein's theories, for example, of the paranoid-schizoid and depressive positions, her conception of sexuality and envy, and her discovery of projective identification as a defense have all been highly influential contributions to the field which, regardless of Klein's intentions, opened up new possibilities for psychoanalysis which were quite different than Freud's classical psychoanalytic practice and theory. The term "object relations" ultimately derived from Klein, since she felt that the infant introjects the 'whole' other with the onset of the depressive position during the ontogenesis of the self.

Klein's student and analysand, Wilfred Bion (1897-1979), has been one of the most influential and gifted of Klein's followers. Bion's work is very complex and difficult to understand, even for one who is well-versed in Kleinian theory. Many of Bion's insights came from his work with schizophrenics, and these observations led him to significantly advance and re-conceptualize Klein's original thinking regarding envy and projective identification. As for envy, Bion felt it involved self-attacks which he called "attacks on linking" designed to sever problematic object relationships, but which, in the end, lead to a destruction of one's good objects. Most importantly, I would argue, Bion's contribution to Kleinian theory is an advancement which moves her theoretically conceived subject out of a solopsistic world of phantasy generated by instinctual drives. In the case of Bion, the mother has a significant impact on the child by the way she assists the child in coping with his or her anxiety. By 'containing' the anxiety of the child, Bion felt, the mother teaches the child to cope with the anxiety. Drawing on this fundamental insight, Bion felt that one of the central tasks of the psychoanalyst is to contain the anxiety of the client. And these process rely on the use of projective identification by which the child or patient projects intolerable anxiety onto the mother or analyst, who in turn 'contains' and gives back to the child the experience in a more manageable form.

British object relations theory, as already mentioned, is indebted to the work of Klein. Interestingly, however, the major figures of object relations theory, including Fairbairn, Winnicott, Balint, Bowlby and Guntrip, developed their positions without taking sides in the debates at the British Psychoanalytic Society. Although billing themselves as "independent" from the traditional Freudians of the "Viennese school" and the Kleinians of the "London school," they were deeply indebted to Klein's work, and, vice versa, Klein was often directly influenced by this "middle school," particularly by Fairbairn.

W. R. D. Fairbairn (1889-1964) dedicated himself to solving the theoretical problems inherent in Freud's hedonic drive theory, which he was never able to reconcile with his observations of the "repetition compulsion." In order to do this, Fairbairn had to reconceptualize Freud's theory of motivation -- thus, the libido. If the libido is primarily pleasure-seeking as Freud has argued, thought Fairbairn, why do people continually involve themselves in traumatic experiences? How can one explain, for example, nightmares, sexual masochism, and traumatic neurosis involving the repetition compulsion? Fairbairn's answer to this riddle is that the libido is not primarily pleasure-seeking, but object-seeking. In other words, intimacy and a connection to others is the primary motivation in human beings and pleasure is rather a secondary motivation derived from this more primary motivation. Also, unlike Klein, internal objects are not inevitable consequences of development, but rather the result of compensations for a real connection with others and stem from disruptions in early object relations with primary caregivers. These insights led Fairbairn to develop a new structure of the psyche which differed from Freud's original tripartite id, ego and superego structure. In particular, Fairbairn conceptualized a "splitting of the ego" into a libidinal and anti-libidinal ego to account for his observations.

D. W. Winnicott (1896-1971) began his career as a pediatrician and used his experience with children to develop his innovative ideas. Like Fairbairn, Winnicott conceptualized the psyche of the child as developing in relation to a real, influential parent. For a child to develop a healthy, genuine self, as opposed to a false self, Winnicott felt, the mother must be a "good-enough mother" who relates to the child with "primary maternal preoccupation." Anticipating the insights of Kohut and self psychology, Winnicott felt that a good-enough mother allows herself to be used by the infant so that he or she may develop a healthy sense of omnipotence which will naturally be frustrated as the child matures. Winnicott's theory is especially innovative regarding his conceptualization of the psychic space between the mother and infant, neither wholly psychological or physical, which he termed the "holding environment" and which allows for the child's transition to being more autonomous. This concept of the "holding environment" led Winnicott to develop his famous theory of the "transitional object." Winnicott felt that a failure of the mother -- the not-good-enough mother -- to provide a "holding environment" would result in a false self disorder, the kind of disorders which he saw in his practice. Winnicott's theory of "false self disorders" is uncannily similar to Laing's description of the schizoid personality in The Divided Self. Winnicott also felt that the therapist's task is to provide such a "holding environment" for the client so that the client might have the opportunity to meet neglected ego needs and allow the true self of the client to emerge.

Other important figures in the Object Relations tradition include Michael Balint, John Bowlby, and Harry Guntrip, as well as the following contributors: Susan Isaac, Hanna Segal, Herbert Rosenfeld, Paula Heimann, Heinrich Racker, Joseph Sandler, Betty Joseph, John Steiner, Elizabeth Bott Spillius, Esther Bick, Thomas Ogden, John D. Sutherland, James Grotstein, Jill Savage Scharff, Otto Kernberg, Nina Coltart, Patrick Casement, Neville Symington, Stephen Mitchell, Christopher Bollas, Henry Ezriel, Henry V. Dick, David E. Scharff, and Elliott Jaques.

For an in depth exploration of Object Relations Theory from an existential-phenomenological perspective, I highly recommend reading an article I've written:

The Paranoid-Schizoid and Depressive Positions in the Psychogenesis of the Self:A Phenomenological Investigation into the Ontological Foundations of Object Relations Theory